Open Access Peer-Reviewed
Artigo de Revisão

A trilogy of the oculomotor system Part I: Ocular movements, finalities, and measurements

A trilogy of the oculomotor system Part I: Ocular movements, finalities, and measurements

Harley E. A. Bicas

DOI: 10.5935/0004-2749.2024-0194

ABSTRACT

The paper starts discussing the teleological concept that eye motions - rotations and translations - serve to vision (which supports the notion that torsions are not voluntarily driven, since they do not contribute to expand the visual exploration of space). It proposes that the primary position of the eye (not "of gaze") , the standard condition to measure them, must be defined as the coincidence of the orbital (fixed) and the ocular (movable) system of coordinates. However this becomes only a theoretic concept, since practical operations to obtain it are almost unfeasible. Besides, even a "simple" horizontal or vertical ocular rotation, though always occurring around a (presumably) fixed point (the center of ocular rotation) may be defined by different trajectories and magnitudes, depending on the two systems of measurement of eye positions and motions. Hence, in a graphical (plane) representation of such spherical coordinates, the so-called "tangent screen", an ocular "tertiary" position - a combination of a horizontal and a vertical rotations - may be described by four different points. Or, conversely, a specific eye position may be defined by four sets of angular coordinates. The mathematical representation of variation of three special coordinates in a specific rotation is best made by a matrix disposition, so that, multiplication (not commutative) of three matrices (one for each specific plane) generates six different systems (permutations) of measurements. So, though , actually, there are multiple trajectories possible between two points in space, the order in which rotations are considered influences the final result. With different systems of coordinates for each rotation and different possible orders by which they may be considered, one reaches 48 alternative systems for their measurements. Unfortunately, up to now, there I is no an established convention to express ocular rotations. So, usually, people consider that a vertical prism superimposed to a frontally placed horizontal prism, or vice-versa, correspond to equivalent processes. The paper finishes discussing inconveniences of the clinically used unity to measure eye rotations (the prism-diopter) and proposes other unities as alternative solutions.

Keywords: Angular measurement unity; eye movements; eye position measurement; eye position measurement accuracy; Fick's system; Helmholtz's system; ocular rotation; primary position of gaze; prism-diopter referential systems; superimposition of prisms

Purpose of eye motions

Vision is the process by which stimulation of the eye by light reflected from objects in space is operated by the brain's perceptual apparatus to create a holistic mental representation of the dimensions, relative positions and other physical qualities of those objects. The initial collection of visual data is the task of an assembled array of photoreceptors that are stimulated by the aforementioned reflected light. However, for photoreceptors to gather this information in a meaningful form, additional complementary factors are needed.

Since the nature of the stimuli (light from a primary source - an emitter - or a secondary one, a reflector) is to propagate in all directions from its origin, a single photoreceptor should be able to receive such information from any object in visual range (Figure 1a). However, its simultaneous stimulation by the light from many objects impedes the discriminative individuation of each. This prevents it, and its fellow photoreceptors, from recording environmental stimuli in a manner that can be perceptually interpreted by the brain. To prevent this information overload, a specific correlation is established between each photoreceptor and a specific spatial area. The coupling of a photoreceptor with a single coordinate excludes any stimulation of that photoreceptor by light from any other point in space (*) (Figure 1b an 1c). Thus, the first complementary factor required for photoreceptors to accurately gather meaningful visual information is a limitation on the stimuli to which each photoreceptor is exposed. Such a limitation is achieved by the establishment of a specific directional correspondence between a photoreceptor and a spatial point in the visual field.

 

 

The second factor necessary for vision is the movement of the sensitive elements (photoreceptors) to avoid a visual field permanently limited to one portion of the environment. In principle, a specific visual direction is taken as a reference for the necessary movements. The line of sight that meets this criterion is the direction of the visual axis, central to the retina, that with the best visual discrimination, the so-called primary (or principal) visual direction, or the visual axis. Ocular motions are described as rotations when the visual axis is rotated around a reference point (the center of ocular rotations). They are described as translations when the position of the reference point is displaced. It can be seen from this that vision is dependent on motion.

 

Reference systems for ocular movements

For mechanical studies, the eye may be considered a relatively rigid, spherical body, surrounded by elastic and viscous matter (muscle fibers, connective tissue, membranes, vitreous fluid, and ligaments). It is held in place by an almost hemispherical cup (the orbit of the skull) that allows the eye to make large, though partial, sliding rotatory movements. During those ocular rotations, the center of mass of the eye is slightly displaced from its original position, but such displacement, as other ocular translations relative to the orbit is small enough that it may be ignored for practical purposes. Thus, the eye may be considered to have a "fixed" center of ocular rotations with a constant position relative to the orbit. The orbital translations and rotations (produced by head movements) play an important role in the visual exploration of space. Therefore, two main referential systems for eye movements may be considered: that for movements of the eye itself (ocular rotations) and that for movements of the orbit (orbital rotations, or translations) with movements of the head.

Since the eye is fixed to the orbit, its geometric center (C) can be used as a fixed point in an orbital reference system. Similarly, the center of mass of the eye (the center of ocular rotations, R) can be used as a fixed reference point in an ocular reference system. In both systems, an imaginary line between the object of visual attention (O) and the fovea (F, sensorial center of the retina) is referred to as the visual axis as it is vital to vision. This line (OF) determines the effective gaze direction. Though points G and R do not necessarily coincide, they are too close that, for practical (clinical) grounds, they may be considered at the same place. Another ideal condition, though not always kept, is achieved when the visual axis coincides with the primary optical axis (an imaginary longitudinal line that passes the dioptric centers of the cornea and the crystalline lens in an anterior-to-posterior direction).

A) The ocular reference system

The visual (y-ocular, or AP-ocular) axis is perpendicular to an imaginary plane containing two other, mutually perpendicular axes, the x-ocular (or LM-ocular); and the z-ocular (or SI-ocular) axis, around which "vertical" and "horizontal" rotations respectively occur. Theses axes are on the vertical and horizontal planes, which exist in these spatial directions relative to the ground (Figure 2).

 

 

By convention, A, S and L in the ocular reference system (see Figure 2, for the left eye) are regarded as "directionally positive". Hence from the perspective at each of those points, clockwise rotations around the respective axis are also seen as "positive". Positive rotations around the vertical, SI-ocular axis are those moving in an A to M direction (adduction). Negative rotations around this axis are those that move in an A to L direction (abduction). Around the transverse, LM-ocular axis, positive rotations are those moving from A to S (sursumduction, or elevation), while negative rotations are those moving from A to I (deorsumduction or depression). Around the longitudinal, AP-ocular axis, positive rotations move from S to L (excycloduction or extorsion), while the counterclockwise (negative) rotations move from S to M (incycloduction or intorsion). Note that these movement descriptions only apply to the left eye. In the right eye, the relative positions of L and M are changed, but the rotation names remain the same (adduction from A to M, elevation from A to S and extorsion from S to L). The positive directions for adduction, elevation and extorsion are now counterclockwise. It is remarkable that cyclorotations (cyclotorsions) around the longitudinal axis (AP), tilt the visual image in a clockwise or a counterclockwise rotation, but do not produce any apparent enlargement of the visual space. This is most likely one of the reasons specific commands from the brain to produce such cyclotorsions are autonomic rather than consciously directed.

B) The orbital reference system

A similar arrangement of mutually perpendicular axes (X, Y, and Z) with a single origin (C) is located on the orbit of the respective eye. The vertical (SI) orbital axis should coincide with the objective vertical line defined by the direction of the gravitational force at the place from where the measurement is considered. This line has to be used to define the objective horizontal plane, to which it is perpendicular. This means that the "true" vertical axis of each orbit are not rigorously parallel, however, the angular difference between them is so slight that it can be ignored for all "practical" purposes. Hence, a vertical plane containing the vertical axis and a horizontal plane perpendicular to the vertical axis can be defined. In practice, clinicians estimate the location of the vertical axis and/or the horizontal plane, by placing the head in a position that allows the vertical and/or horizontal lines of the human face to be so observed. Given the relative anatomical symmetry of the head and facial features and their approximate coincidence with the objective horizontal plane and the sagittal plane, the orbits will be positioned to allow estimation of the "objective" vertical axis (and/or horizontal plane) when the head is central and not tilted toward the right or to the left shoulders (an assumption of the required horizontal plane adjustment).

The common orthogonality of the three planes (and their respective axes) can now be used to define the frontal plane and/or horizontal lines (a transversal line contained by that plane, or a longitudinal one perpendicular to it). The "natural" (anatomical) orbital axis is a line that extends from the front to the apex of the orbit. However, it does not fulfill the criteria for what it is known as the progressive phylogenetical anteriority of the orbits, to support the concept of the human binocular vision - a fundamental requirement for the study of eye positions and movements - as the consequence of the superimposed visual fields. Therefore, the orbital frontal planes are conventionally defined by the (horizontal) LM-orbital axes, an imaginary transversal line from the right to the left sides of the head (so that both, the sagittal --- vertical) orbital planes can be considered parallel. When the subject is facing front and not turned to the right or left, one can infer the correct adjustment of the frontal plane of the orbits.

Once these orbital objective planes and their respective axes have been defined, they can be made coincident to the respective planes and axes of the eye, giving eye "positions" and rotations a fixed (to the head) and objective system of references for their measurements. The position of the visual axis at such a coincidence of the orbital and corresponding ocular axes is regarded as the primary position of the respective eye and the landmark used in measurements of (other) eye positions and movements.

C) The primary position of the eye

The simplicity of the concept of a primary position of the eye as the complete coincidence of the respective axes of the eye and the orbit poses some disturbing comments:

1) Unfortunately, despite this quite simple theoretical definition of the primary position of the eye, the practical operations by which it may be obtained are complex and near impossible to work out. Furthermore, there is a lack of standards with which to objectify orbital planes (such as a plumb line to define the vertical and horizontal planes) and obstacles to applying guidelines in practice (facial asymmetries, though sometimes minute, are always present, and these interfere with the perpendicular relationship between the expected horizontal and vertical reference lines). This makes any preemptive affirmation that the head position is perfectly adjusted to an objective horizontal plane imprudent. Nonetheless, possible conventions have been proposed(1). To summarize, approximate of the frontal plane of the head to an acceptably symmetrical position (from the perspective of the clinician) and of the sagittal plane of the head to ensure it is not inclined relative to the objective vertical line, can be clinically satisfactory. However, the lack of established standards for improved accuracy of such approximations warrants further discussion. The major barrier to the development of such standards is determining the correct adjustment of the horizontal plane of the head. An "erect" front-facing head position is not easy to achieve precisely. This is apparent when one attempts it in front of a mirror. The reader may wish to attempt this and try to identify the correct balance point (i.e., that corresponding to the objective, horizontal plane) among a relatively extended range of more "haughty" or "depressed" head positions.

2) The primary position of gaze has been proposed as a reference direction for the measurement of eye positions and movements. In such an instance, "gaze" is perhaps not the best descriptor. Gaze generally refers to direction of the visual axis (attentively or not) toward a particular spatial point. As the eye may be rotated around a given direction (as, for example, the direction of the visual axis), innumerable eye positions (torsions) are possible-with-the-same direction of gaze. Rigorous measurements of ocular torsion cannot be guaranteed, since only the longitudinal axis can be defined using proper referential conditions. The other ocular axes (transverse and sagittal) cannot. Also, ocular torsion may be only measured using information given by the owner of the eye under evaluation. Consequently, a specific position of the eye cannot be determined using only the direction of the longitudinal (visual) axis.

3) Similarly, the adjustment of a pair of equivalent axes from each monocular system (for instance, the longitudinal, or sagittal axes of both eyes) is insufficient to define the primary position of the eye. The coincidence of the transverse (horizontal) axes of both eyes does not mean that the respective vertical axes are parallel.

4) The origin of the orbital axes (C) should not be taken to be the center of the orbit, nor the (geometrical) center of the eye. While it is not necessary for measurements of eye movements (among which ocular rotations prevail), it is more convenient C be made to coincide with R, the center of ocular rotations.

5) The permanent coincidence of the ocular and orbital axis, that is, the fixed position of the eye relative to the orbit is a theoretical simplification used in the study of ocular rotations. But it has previously been shown that such a conceptual point would be displaced (translated) during the rotations. At best, the visual axis (or "line of sight") may rotate around a fixed point in space (orbit) without passing by R(2,3).

6) Since the center of ocular rotation (R) and the geometric center of the eye (G) are not equivalent (Figure 3), points on the scleral surface (such as the insertion points of muscular fibers) are equidistant from G, but not from R. For instance, while the respective coordinates of M and L have the same distance to G and maintain this distance during eye rotations, they have different values when considered relatively to R (Figure 3). This is because, even in a perfectly spherical eye, the rotational arms of the insertions for each muscle fiber (or the rotational "radius of the eye") are unequal.

 

 

Measurements of eye rotations

As a matter of simplification, the eye may be considered as a perfect sphere, which geometric center is the center of its rotation (C). All of its superficial points are, then, equidistant from C and rest over imaginary "large" circles (e.g., the equator or any of its perpendicular sections, the circles of longitudes). Except for the "poles" all other points of the ocular surface may be also defined as resting over imaginary "small" circles (as those of "latitudes", that is, circles parallel to a considered equator). Therefore, similarly to any point of the terrestrial surface, superficial points of a sphere may have a coordinate of "longitude" and one of "latitude". All the same, a sphere can be divided by curved (and parallel) lines of "small" circles (lines of "latitudes") and be divided by lines of "large" circles (lines of longitudes), mutually perpendicular. For the case of the eye, although all ocular rotations occur, always, around C, the trajectory of different points of the ocular surface describe arcs of different magnitudes, all of them centered at a normal (perpendicular) axis to the plane of the respective rotation.

For instance, a measurement of a horizontal angle ("H") may be measured around a fixed vertical axis (CS) so that arcs in different planes have different lengths (AB or DF, Figure 4a). But if the angles are measured relatively to the center of rotation (C), the rotational arcs have the same length AB and DG, Figure 4a).

 

 

All the same, the measurement of a vertical angle may be given by the same pair of conventions. If they are considered around a fixed transversal axis (CL, Figure 4b), the arcs have different lengths (AD and BG) but correspond to the same angle, defined as "V". Arcs measured from C have the same length (BF and AD) and are defined as "v". Angle measurements taken from the center of ocular rotations ("h" and "v") are said to be of the ocular system of coordinates. Those measured from points of fixed axes in space ("H" and "V") are said to be of the orbital system of coordinates.

Therefore, even if one consider only a "simple" rotation, taken from the primary position in a fundamental (horizontal or vertical) plane, a point may have two trajectories and two final positions. For instance, a vertical rotation of 30º for the point B is the arc BF if one considers "v", but the arc BG if one considers "V". All the same, a horizontal rotation of 30º for point D means arc DG if one considers "h", or arc DF if one considers "H".

When one consider the case of both, vertical and horizontal angles with the same value (say 30º), four different combinations result (Figures 4a and 4b): "HV", if both measurements are related to the orbital coordinates (point I); "Hv", if the horizontal measurement is related to the orbital system and the vertical measurement is related to the ocular system of coordinates (point F), or vice-versa ("hV", point G), or "hv" if both measurements are related to the ocular system of coordinates (point E). Conversely, since the same set of angular coordinates corresponds to four different positions of a point, the position of a point may correspond to four different sets of coordinates, that is, HV, Hv, hV or hv (Figure 4c).

If one considers the three (horizontal, vertical and torsional) rotations, the possible combinations become eight (HVT, HVt, HvT, Hvt, hVT, hVt, hvT, hvt).

a) Consider Figure 4a as similar to the terrestrial system of coordinates, with line AB as the equator and AD as the meridian of a standard longitude. F is then defined by a longitude (the horizontal angle) ACB = DZF, and a latitude BCF (the vertical angle) = ACD. This system of coordinates for measurement of horizontal rotations (as "longitudes") and vertical rotations (as "latitudes") is that of Fick.

b) The system whereby vertical rotations "are measured using the angle of elevation" and horizontal rotations "using the angle of Azimuth" (coordinates for point G, Figure 4b) is that of Helmholtz.

Planar representation of spherical coordinates

As shown in Figure 5, the spherical arcs and points of a sphere may be represented on a flat plane, perpendicular to one of its main axes, using their projections from the respective center of reference. In ocular rotation measurement, this would be the longitudinal axis (the visual axis) (Figure 3). The projections from this are called central, polar, zenithal or gnomonic projections. The flat plane, supposed to tangentially touch the anterior pole of the eye (A) is called a tangent screen. In the case depicted in Figure 6, the positions of the anterior pole of the sphere (A) and its representation on a flat plane (A') are coincident and serve as the origin of a system of (planar) coordinates. In such a plane, the distance between any projected point (Pp) and its origin (Ap = A), distance PpAp (= PpA) is given by a straight line perpendicular to the line between this origin (Ap) and the center of ocular rotations (C), which is the radius of curvature of the sphere (r). In other words, PpAp is related to constant (r) by a simple tangent scale. For example, for point K (and its projection Kp), ApKp = r tan a = [(ApJp)2 + (JpKp)2]½.

 

 

 

 

As can be seen in Figure 6, even when the sphere does not touch the flat plane (screen), but is separated from it by distance AAp = d, the tangent scale still holds: ApKp = k tan a, where k = r + d. This is the reason for the use of the general term "tangent screen" in the figures showing measurements that use this type of gnomonic projection.

Figure 7 provides a graphical representation of the points from Figure 5 that could be represented by the same set of angular coordinates on a flat plane of gnomonic projections. Note that, in a gnomonic projection, the curved lines of large circles ("the equator" and "meridians") are represented by straight lines, while those of small circles (parallel to large circles) are represented by hyperbolic curves (Figure 8).

 

 

 

 

Mathematical presentation of different systems of ocular rotations

We have now seen that although all ocular rotations (whether horizontal or vertical) are effectively centered at C (the center of ocular rotations), as "h" or "v", the coordinate system used to determine the position of a point on the ocular surface may be defined by other referential concepts (as "H" or "V"). Using the different criteria of each system to calculate the coordinates of the point in question (e.g., point E in Figure 7), four combinatioons emerge (HV, Hv, hV and hv).

It has been also shown that betweeen two points (e.g. A and E in Figures 4 to 8), infinite paths are possible (for instance from A to D and D to E); or from A to B and B to E; or directly from A to E; or any other). That is, the coordinates of a point (E) are not dependent on the temporal order in which rotations are performed. Note, however, that for a given point (e.g., point E, as shown in Figures 5 and 7), H < h, and V < v, so that establishing a criterion for measurement of a horizontal rotation (H or h) influences the "subsequent" measurement of the vertical rotation (V or v), and vice-versa. For instance, if one defines a horizontal rotation of 30º as "h", "H" will be < 30º.

Let us suppose that all rotations on different planes (Figure 9) are ascertained from the same center (C).

 

 

In Figure 9, a + b + # = 90º (where # represents h, v or –t) so that

sin (a + b) = sin (90º– h) = cos # = (sin a)(cos b) + (cos a)(sin b)

cos (a + b) = cos (90°–h ) = sin # = (cos a)(cos b) – (sin a)(sin b)

But in Figure 9a, CPi = CPf = k, so:

sin a = yi / k cos a = xi / k sin b = xf / k cos b = yf / k

Hence:

cos h = (yi / k) (yf / k) + (xi / k) (xf / k) = [ (xi)(xf) + (yi) (yf) ] / k2 (F. I)

sin h = (xi / k) (yf / k) – (yi / k) (xf / k) = [ (xi)(yf) – (yi) (xf) ] / k2 (F. II)

Isolating yf from F. I and F. II:

yf = [ k2 (cos h) – (xi)(xf) ] / yi = [ k2 (sin h) + (yi) (xf) ] / xi

(xi) [ k2 (cos h) – (xi)(xf) ] = (yi) [ k2 (sin h) + (yi) (xf) ] →

k2 [xi (cos h) – yi (sin h)] = xf [ (yi)2 + (xi)2 ]

However, (yi)2 + (xi)2 = (yf)2 + (xf)2 = k2, so that:

xi (cos h) – yi (sin h) = xf (F.III)

Alternatively, isolation of xf from F. I and F. II:

xf = [ k2 (cos h) – (yi)(yf) ] / xi = [ (xi) (yf) – k2 (sin h) ] / yi

(yi) [ k2 (cos h) – (yi)(yf) ] = (xi) [ (xi) (yf) – k2 (sin h) ] →

k2 [yi (cos h) + xi (sin h)] = yf [ (xi)2 + (yi)2 ] →

yi (cos h) + xi (sin h) = yf (F. IV)

Similarly, in Figure 9b (changing "h" to "v", "x" to "y" and "y" to "z"), equations F.III and F. IV become:

yi (cos v) – zi (sin v) = yf(F.V)

zi (cos v) + yi (sin v) = zf (F. VI)

Finally, in Figure 9c, a + b = 90 – (– t), hence:

sin (a + b) = sin (90 + t) = cos t = (sin a) (cos b) + (sin b) (cos a) →

cos t = (zi / k) (zf / k) + (xf / k) (xi / k) = [ (zi) (zf) + (xi) (xf) ] / k2 (F. VII)

cos (a + b) = cos (90 + t) = –sin t = (cos a) (cos b) – (sin a) (sin b) =

– sin t = (xi / k) (zf / k) – (zi / k) (xf / k) → sin t = [(xf) (zi) – (xi) (zf)] / k2 (F. VIII)

By equalization of zf in F. VII and F. VIII:

[k2 (cos t) – (xi) (xf)] / zi = zf = –[k2 (sin t) – (xf) (zi)] / xi

k2(cos t) xi – (xi)2 (xf) = –k2(sin t) zi + (xf)(zi)2

k2 [(cos t) xi + zi (sin t)] = (xf) [(zi)2 + (xi)2] →

xf = xi cos t + zi sin t (F. IX)

And, by equalization of xf in F. VII and F. VIII:

[k2 (cos t) – (zf) (zi)] / (xi) = xf = [k2 (sin t) + (xi) (zf)] / (zi)] →

k2 (cos t) (zi) – (zf) (zi)2 = k2 (sin t) (xi) + (zf) (xi)2

k2 [(cos t) (zi) – (sin t) (xi)] = (zf) [(xi)2 + (zi)2] →

zf = – (xi) sin t + (zi) cos t (F. X)

Equations F. III and F. IV may be represented in matrix form:

As can equations F. V and F. VI:

And equations F. IX and F. X:

Matrices (Mh) , (Mv) and, or (Mt) may be multiplied to produce different results. The products of matrices are not commutative, so the order in which they are multiplied alters the results. For matrices (Mh) and (Mv), one may have:

Therefore, when two different rotations are considered (for example of 30º and 40º) it is essential to know which of them (h or v) is = 30º. Of course, h = 30º (so v= 40º) is absolutely different from v = 30º (so h = 40º).

For example, for point P (x, y, z), the coordinates are named L (1, 0, 0) for x, A (0, 1, 0) for y and as S (0, 0, 1) for z, so P (L, A, S):

For cases where h = –90º (abduction) and v = 0º , xf = A, yf = –L, zf = S. For h = 0º and v = 90º, xf = L, yf = –S , zf = A. And for h = –90º and v = 90, xf = –S, yf = –L, and zf = A. The initial and the final positions of points L, A, and S are visualized in Figure 10 (above).

 

 

However, for the product of matrices (Mv).(Mh) the results are quite different. Using the same set of coordinates for point P (L, A, S):

Therefore, for h = –90º and v = 0º, xf = A, yf = –L , zf = S. For h = 0º and v = 90º, xf = L, yf = –S, zf = A. For v = 90º and h = –90º, xf = A , yf = –S, zf = –L (Figure 10, below).

Obviously, after a pure horizontal rotation, that is, if h = -90º (v = 0º), the final positions reached by the ocular axes are the same, independent of the product of matrices (F.XVI or F.XVII) used, that is xf = A, yf = - L, zf = S (Figure 10, above). Similarly, after a pure vertical rotation, that is, if v = 90º (h = 0º), the final positions of the ocular coordinates for A, L, and S are the same, independent of the product of matrices (F. XVI, or F. XVII) used, that is, xf = L, yf = -S, zf = A (Figure 10, below). But even when the values of angular coordinates are the same (h = –90º, v = 90º), different results emerge, according to the multiplication of matrices that is used (F.XVI or F.XVII), as Figure 10 shows. Of course, this could already be anticipated based on Figure 5. If one utilizes the angle measurements from the Fick system, point A (with the vertical rotation from B to F " calculated after" the horizontal rotation from A to B, both around C) reaches the original position of S (Figure 10, above). If one uses the Helmholtz system, point A (with the horizontal rotation from D to G "calculated after" the vertical rotation, from A to D, both around C) reaches the original position of L (Figure 10, below). In fact, ocular rotations are always around C (or very close to an ideally fixed point in space) so it is merely due to the conventions for the expression of the angle coordinates that they may be taken to occur around different points (e.g., Z for F and X for G, Figure 5).

As a result of the several modes with which one may consider the angle coordinates used to measure an eye rotation on a plane (horizontal, sagittal or frontal): whether they occur at the center of ocular rotations and around movable (ocular) axes (h, v, and t, respectively) or around points on a fixed (orbital) referential axis (H, V, and T, respectively); and the order in which they are calculated, there are 48 possible combinations of these conventions (Table 1). However, the representation of eye positions by a system of polar coordinates does not preclude the knowledge of the order in which horizontal (h) and vertical (v) rotations are measured.

 

 

The need for a specific order of calculation had already been posited when coordinate systems for eye rotation measurements were first proposed(4). Surprisingly, there remains no established and agreed-upon convention that has been shown preferable among the possible approaches to the measurement of ocular rotations. Since such rotations occur around a known center, it seems logical to select one of the six arrangements of measurements h, v, and t, shown in the last row of table 1.

While horizontal and vertical rotations are volitional and can be of relatively large magnitudes, torsional rotations are small and not subject to voluntary control. This lack of hierarchical and teleological importance (torsional rotations do not expand the oculomotor fields) suggests that in the measurement of ocular rotations, torsions (t) should be calculated last. If a choice must be made among the alternatives in the last row of table 1, then "hvt" or "vht" should prevail. Strictly, however, these cannot be taken as the representations of the Fick (Hvt) or Helmholtz (Vht) systems(*).

 

RETROSPECTIVE SYNTHESIS

Finalities of vision (indirect contact with, and exploration of space) are essentially dependent on eye motions.

The complex functionality of vision is almost entirely dependent on movements. The delicate and motionless assembly of structures (retina) for capturing stimulation from the environment (light) to transform it in a mentally recorded "picture" benefits of statically concerted ("centered") optical elements. But if such an ideally rigid construction (eye) were absolutely immovable the result would be of a limited image. Bodies could be seen traversing this framed space, though not being followed.

Eye movements are of two types: translations, when the eye as a whole - e.g., a "frozen" (immovable) eye - is spatially displaced by head movements; and rotations, when only one ocular point (its center of rotation) remain fixed, while all others are displaced relatively to the respective container (orbit). Although both of them are very important for the visual exploration of space, in clinical practice is usual to consider ocular movements as synonymous of ocular rotations.

Ocular rotations may occur in any direction of space, in a specific plane and around an imaginary axis perpendicular to it. They are formally defined according to the three, mutually orthogonal, spatial axes: one vertical (perpendicular to the horizontal plane and around which are defined the horizontal rotations) and two other horizontal axes, one perpendicular to the sagittal plane (the transversal ocular axis, around which are defined the vertical rotations) and another perpendicular to the frontal plane (the longitudinal ocular axis, around which are defined the torsional rotations). Exploration of the space is largely dependent on horizontal and vertical rotations which, therefore, may be voluntarily driven, while torsions do not contribute to enlarge the "visual fields", so that usually small and reflexes to complement the former, volitionally commanded, eye rotations. Measurements of eye rotations may refer either to the ocular (movable) or to the orbital (fixed) system of coordinates.

The primary position of gaze is the landmark from where the measurements of eye positions and, or ocular rotations are made. This is conventionally defined as the (spatial) "placement" of the visual axis, the longitudinal (anterior to posterior) ocular axis, when the three respective axes of each (ocular and orbital) reference systems coincide. Note that "gaze straight ahead" - a construction frequently used to define the "primary position of gaze" - does not fulfill, necessarily, such a conception. Although "gaze" refers to "vision" (that is, the "position" or "direction" of the ocular longitudinal axis), while "straight ahead" address to "head" (that is, perpendicularly to the orbital frontal plane), that means that the ocular and the orbital longitudinal axes are coincident (gaze straight ahead), but the other two ocular axes (vertical and transversal) may be rotated (with torsion) relatively to the respective orbital axes.

Measurements of ocular rotations may be considered accordingly the two (ocular or orbital) reference system of coordinates. Except for the ocular poles at the primary position of gaze, any other point of the ocular surface is represented by different sets of coordinates. For instance (follow figure 5), for measurements of angular horizontal coordinates, the position of a specific point (E) may be expressed relatively to the vertical (orbital, fixed) reference system by an angle of "longitude" measured from a standard meridian (AS, that of the orbital sagittal plane) on an arc of "small" circle (D'Z'E) of its "latitude", centered at point Z' of the vertical (orbital, fixed) axis of reference. But while this measurement may be labelled as "H" (equal to ACB') a different angle measurement (‘h"), taken from the center of ocular rotation (C), may be also defined for the same point (angle DCE = angle ACB. All the same, for measurements of angular vertical coordinates, the same point E may be defined either by a value "V" (angle B'X'E = ACD') or by a value "v" (angle BCE = ACD). Therefore, if E may be defined by two different horizontal angular coordinates (H or h, where h > H) and, or by two different vertical angular coordinates (V or v, where v > V), it may have four different combinations of vertical and horizontal coordinates (HV, Hv, hV and hv). If torsion is also considered, the same point have two different torsional measurements, accordingly the reference system elected (T or t), so that eight different set of coordinates result for defining the same point (HVT, HVt, HvT, Hvt, hVT, hVt, hvT, hvt). Hence, if the same point may be defined by different angular coordinates, conversely, the same set of coordinates may correspond to different points.

Besides the two (orbital, and ocular) system of coordinates with which the position of a point on the ocular surface (or of its projection) and, or of its trajectory may be defined, the order with which the rotations may be considered bring possible six possible arrangements (e.g., hvt, htv, vht, vth, thv, tvh). In fact, this corresponds to the order of how the correspondent matrix equations of each rotation in a plane are multiplied. As, for each arrangement (order) two different systems of coordinates are possible for defining each rotation, 48 alternatives result.

 

OPERATION OF MEASUREMENTS

The orthogonal superimposition of horizontal and vertical prims

The choice of a system of spherical coordinates and angle measurements has an important implication. In clinical practice, angle measurements of eye deviations (strabismus) are made using prisms. It is well known that such measurements can vary greatly depending on the position relatively to the eye that incident and emergent faces are. Therefore, specific rules must be followed when making these prismatic measurements. The preferred rule is probably the placement of the prism in a position, such that the face from which the light emerges coincides with the frontal plane of the respective orbit. (References to the positions on a prism "base", as "nasal", "temporal", "superior" or "inferior" are merely conceptual and not technically valid. In fact, the meaning of "base" is simply of a "side"- it does not imply a "face". Besides, if a plane face exists as a "base", it will not be a guide for the prism placement before the eye.) The greater the angle to be measured, the greater the apical angle of the prism. If a combination of horizontal and vertical measurements is necessary, an orthogonal superimposition of prisms can be used.

Although the relative positions of the horizontal and vertical prisms (i.e., which of them is placed coincident to the frontal plane of the orbit), are rarely considered, differences in this relationship do exist as shown in Figures 11 and 12. If the horizontal prism is used "first" to the eye, the arrangement corresponds to the "hv(t)" system; if the vertical prism is used "first" to the eye, it corresponds to the "vh(t)" system. Obviously, this means that the way an arrangement of a horizontal and a vertical prisms is made before the eye affects the calculated magnitudes of deviations. (Figures 11 and 12 use in extremis illustrations of theoretical cases of prisms with apical angles of 90º. They are not intended to suggest that these angles occur.)

 

 

 

 

The accuracy and the significance of measurements

The evaluation of ocular movements is limited by two natural conditions, one anatomical and one mechanical. The anatomical limitation is the size of the smallest photoreceptor field at the center of the fovea (2 μm) of a human eye when measured from the second nodal point of the eye (17.055 mm before it, according to the classical Gullstrand's reference)(5) and corresponds to angle (a) given by arctan a = 2.10-6 m / 17.055.10-3 m, hence a ≈0.0067º ≈24.2". This is in fair agreement with the discriminative limit of the optical ocular system, given by the radius size of the first circle of diffraction (Airy's disc)(6): A = 1.22 λ / d = 1.22 . 550.10-9 / 6.10-3 ≈ 112.10-6rad ≈ 23.1" where λ is the wavelength of yellow light (550 nm) and d is the diameter of a "normal" pupil. Hence, the distance between two points that can be optically distinguished from one another, is twice the value of angle A, about 48" or 0.8'. However, this is an optical condition, and ocular rotations are measured from their rotational center, closer to the fovea (about 12.2 mm) than the second nodal point of the optical system. So the mechanical limiting condition is that of angle "e" estimated by 17 mm / 12,2 mm = e / 48", that is, e ≈ 67" ≈ 1.1'.

The eye does not rest immobile but rather, is continuously in motion due to very fine oscillatory movements. These are categorized as high-frequency tremors (which may reach 1'), slow drifts (about 5'), rapid "flicks" or saccades (up to 20') and other irregular movements (up to 5')(7). Together these occur within a retinal area about 100 μm in diameter(7) (which corresponds to about 0.47º or 28' from the center of ocular rotation). This is in accord with experiments concerned with the physiological evocation of a minimal ocular response: displacements smaller than 15' to 30' were not capable of eliciting saccades(8). It is commonly accepted that is difficult, if not impossible, even for an experienced ophthalmic clinician, to detect an ocular deviation of such an order of magnitude (0.5º) with the naked eye.

Scales and unities

Angles may be quantified as degrees of an arc (an arc of 1º is 1/360 of a circumference) but also as radians. An arc of 1 rad has a length equal to the radius of the circumference. Hence 360º = 2 Π radians, or 1 rad = 180º/Π ≈ 57.296º ≈ 3437.747'(*). For measurements of the order of strabismus deviations, the centesimal part of the radian (the cent-rad) has been proposed as the unit of measurement(9).

However, the exact measurement of a curved line, such as the arc of a circle, presents practical difficulties. In 1890, Prentice proposed an estimative of angle measurements based on a simple relationship between two straight and perpendicular lines (Figure 13).(10) This is calculated by determining the distance between two points (AB) and the distance between one of these points (say, A) and the point from which they are observed (C). The measurement unit, then defined as 100 AB/AC, was named prism-diopter. A small superscripted triangle is used to represent this unit (Δ). The benefits of such a simple measurement are evident. If one knows the distance between two points (say 2 m, which is the distance which separates a doubled image of an object) and the distance from which they are observed (say, 10 m), the ratio 100 x (2/10) = 20 represents the angle in prism-diopters (what is C - if the first principal point of the eye, the center of ocular rotation, or the fovea - has not been indicated? For clinical purposes, the possible differences related to distance AC are so small, relatively, as to be irrelevant).

 

 

The corresponding angle of 1Δ = 1 cm/ 1 m = 0.01, that is, tan a = 0.01, leads to a ≈ 0.572938697º. The value of this angle (a) is almost "exactly" the same as that of a cent-rad (1 cent-rad = 1.8 / Π ≈ 0.572957795º a difference of 0.003333 % ≈ 1/30000), which allows an acceptable estimate of 4º ≈ 6.98 cent-rad ≈ 7Δ. The major problem is that while this direct ("linear") approximation holds for relatively small angle values, it does not allow arithmetic operations with prism-diopter unities. For example, 2 x 40º = 80º. But 40º (≈83.91Δ) + 40º (≈ 83.91Δ) = 80º (≈ 567.13Δ). A related question concerns the superimposition of prisms, for which the analysis is still more complex. Two prisms with n = 1.49 and 40Δ, superimposed so their apices are coincident, evoke an angle deviation of 337.87Δ(11).

What is known as the practical rule of Prentice, i.e., using a ratio of straight lines (two orthogonal distances, x and d) to express angle measurements, is actually a tangent scale, since a measurement in prism-diopters (P) is PΔ = 100 tan (x/d), which requires the rather disturbing conversion of a 90º angle to an infinite value in prism-diopters. Crescent angle values of 90º to 180º are converted to decreasing and negative values in prism-diopters. However, a new way of defining the same prism-diopter unity (Figure 13) changes such discrepancies enormously. An angle of 90º becomes 200 u and crescent angle values of 90º to 180º increase in a positive direction (up to an infinite value for the expression of 180º). Mathematically, the angle measurement (U) with the new unity is simply given by(12):

U = 200 tan (x/2 d) (F. XVIII)

An additional theoretical advantage of this convention is that it conforms to the existing practical convention used to define a prism angle value by evoking its minimum deviation. This angle of minimum deviation is precisely produced by the symmetrical positioning of the incident and refracted rays relative to the optical surfaces of the prism (Figure 14, left) equivalent to their symmetrical positions relative to the object and the observation point. This is similar to the "split" position used to define the new angular unity (Figure 13).

 

 

If the new scale (U) is used with a prism previously found to be 50 cent-radians (≈28.648º) and 54.630 prism-diopters, the value in this new scale drops to 51.068 u. Still better approximations may be reached, if the "divide and multiply" criteria are used(13). For instance, the value becomes 50.262 unities if one defines the unity in the new scale (U4) using the formula U4 = 400 tan (a/4), or 50.002 unities if the formula is U50 = 5000 tan (a/50).

The exact relationship between a ratio of two orthogonal distances (x and d), a tangent scale, which provides the basic principle for defining prism-diopter (P) or unities at this "split" scale (U), as an angle measurement in degrees of arc (or radians), may be perfectly reached using the equation

Uk = (100 k) tan (a / k) (F. XIX)

when k = ∞. If, for instance, k = 10, an angle (a) of 90º (= 157.080 cent-radians) can be converted to U10 = 1000 tan 9º = 158.384 unities, with an error of only 0.83% relative to the angle value in cent-radians. Table 2 shows angle values in degrees of arc, the corresponding conversions to cent-radians, the new angular unity values (Uk) according to the chosen convention for its calculation (k) and the respective percent errors (e), where e = 100 (Uk – c) / c.

 

 

Even though an angle of 45º is too large to occur much in the measurement of eye rotations, and deviations, in clinical practice, the most basic "split" unity (U2, shown in figure 13), can measure it with an "absolute" error (e), where e = 100 (U2 – c) / c of only about 5% (i.e., 82.84271 / 78.53982 ≈ 1,0548).

 

REFERENCES

1. Bicas HEA. Strabismus: from theory to practice, from concepts to its operational attainment. Arq Bras Oftalmol. 2009;72(5):585-615.

2. Park RS, Park GE. The center of ocular rotation in the horizontal plane. Am J Physiol. 1933;104:545-552.

3. Fry GA, Hill WW. The center of rotation of the eye. Am J Optom. 1962;39:581-595.

4. Bicas HEA. The Laws and Rationales of the Oculomotor Balance. Conference I.S.A. – C.L.A.D.E. In Strabimsus 2006. Proceedings of the Joint Congress The Xth Meeting of the International Strabismological Association and the First Extraordinary Meeting of the Latinamerican Council of Strabismus. Rio de Janeiro: Cultura Médica, pp.39-68.

5. Gullstrand A. Appendices to Part I. In: Southall JPC, editor. Helmholtz's Treatise on Physiological Optics. Rochester: The Optical Society of America; 1924. p.352.

6. Fincham WHA. Optics. 7th ed. London: Hatton Press; 1965.

7. Alpern M. Types of movement. In: The eye. Vol. 3 Muscular Mechanisms. Davson H, edit., 2nd ed. New York: Academic Press; 1969. p.65-174.

8. Rashbass C. The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements. J Physiol. 1961;159(2):326-338.

9. Dennet WS. A new method of numbering prisms. Tr Am Ophthalmol Soc. 1889;5:422-6.

10. Prentice CF. A metric system of numbering and measuring prisms. Arch Ophthalmol. 1890;19:64-75.

11. Bicas HEA. Efeitos rotacionais mono e binoculares das associações de prismas. Rev Bras Oftalmol, 1980;39(1):33-45.

12. Bicas HEA. Unidades de medidas. In: Bicas HEA, Souza-Dias C, Almeida H, eds. Estrabismos. Rio de Janeiro: Cultura Médica; 2007. p.69-72.

13. Bicas HEA. A new unity for angular measurements in strabismus. Arq Bras Oftalmol, 2014;77(5):275-9.

 

*The relationships between photoreceptors (as represented by T, F, N) and points in space (represented by V, B, G) are given by specific "lines of sight" passing by an imaginary point (H), the optical center of the eye (between the two nodal points of the optical ocular system). However, the area of each photoreceptor have infinite points, each of them correlating to a specific point in space, so that the infinite "lines of sight" corresponding to each photoreceptor are, actually, best represented by the figure of two cylinders with apexes at H. They are mentally converted into a single (compacted) relativized direction.

Submitted for publication: June 27, 2024.
Accepted for publication: June 28, 2024.

Funding: This study received no specific financial support.

Disclosure of potential conflicts of interest: The author declare no conflicts of interest.


Dimension

© 2024 - All rights reserved - Conselho Brasileiro de Oftalmologia